{"created":"2023-06-20T14:55:07.237909+00:00","id":70,"links":{},"metadata":{"_buckets":{"deposit":"0f4be4e4-01db-4426-a5c1-68d3d12007d7"},"_deposit":{"created_by":13,"id":"70","owners":[13],"pid":{"revision_id":0,"type":"depid","value":"70"},"status":"published"},"_oai":{"id":"oai:klc.repo.nii.ac.jp:00000070","sets":["1:3"]},"author_link":["220","217","219","223","218","222","224","221"],"item_10002_biblio_info_7":{"attribute_name":"書誌情報","attribute_value_mlt":[{"bibliographicIssueDates":{"bibliographicIssueDate":"2003-07-31","bibliographicIssueDateType":"Issued"},"bibliographicPageEnd":"10","bibliographicPageStart":"1","bibliographicVolumeNumber":"30","bibliographic_titles":[{"bibliographic_title":"紀要visio : research reports"},{"bibliographic_title":"Visio"}]}]},"item_10002_description_5":{"attribute_name":"抄録","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_description":"本研究では,互恵的利他行動の文脈における裏切り者の名前が,再発チェックのために効率的に検索されるのかどうかを検討した。16名の大学生が2セッションからなる実験に参加した。被験者には,第1セッションで互恵的利他行動が破綻する文章を読ませた。文章には,裏切り者と蔓切られる者,利害には無関係の中立者の3名が登場した(利害変数)。また,この文章読解では,被験者自身の名前が裏切られる者として登場するかどうかによって関与の有無を操作した(関与変数)。第2セッションでは,先の文章に登場した人物名かどうかを判断して選択的に反応する記憶探索課題を行った。もし裏切り者の検索過程が再発チェックのために効率化されるならば,その反応時間は少なくとも中立条件よりも短くなるはずであり,その程度は被験者自身に損害が及ぶと想定される場合の方が顕著であると予想された。実験の結果,関与有り条件では裏切り者に対する反応時間が中立条件よりも有意に短かったが,そのような差は関与無し条件では認められなかった。この結果は,先の予想を支持するものであった。","subitem_description_type":"Abstract"}]},"item_10002_description_6":{"attribute_name":"内容記述","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_description":"We examined whether in a context of reciprocal altruism (Trivers, 1971) the name of the imaginary cheater is efficiently retrieved because participants regulate their memory to check the behavior of the cheater. Sixteen graduate students participated in 2 parts of this experiment. In the first section, the subjects were required to read a passage in which the reciprocity of altruistic behaviors failed because of a cheating behavior. The characters in the passage were a cheater, a cheated person, and a neutral person (the interest variable). In addition, 2 conditions were provided concerning whether the subject's name was present in the passage as the cheated person or not (the participation variable). In the second section, the subjects performed a memory search task in which they selectively responded according to whether the name stimulus, which was presented on a display, was shown in the passage that they had previously read. If the cheater's name is efficiently retrieved because participants are regulating their memory for the aim of checking the behavior of the cheater, then the reaction time for the cheater's name should be shorter than that for the neutral one. Furthermore, such a difference in reaction time should be greater in the participation condition in which subjects themselves suffer damage compared to the non-participation condition. Results showed that the reaction time for the cheater's name in the participation condition was significantly shorter than that for the neutral person's name, but it was not so in the non-participation condition. Therefore these results support the above hypothesis.","subitem_description_type":"Other"}]},"item_10002_full_name_3":{"attribute_name":"著者別名","attribute_value_mlt":[{"nameIdentifiers":[{}],"names":[{"name":"Iwaki, Nobuyoshi"}]},{"nameIdentifiers":[{}],"names":[{"name":"Kosaka, Keiko"}]},{"nameIdentifiers":[{}],"names":[{"name":"Kondou, Takeo"}]},{"nameIdentifiers":[{}],"names":[{"name":"Habuchi, Yoshiko"}]}]},"item_10002_publisher_8":{"attribute_name":"出版者","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_publisher":"九州ルーテル学院大学"}]},"item_10002_source_id_9":{"attribute_name":"ISSN","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_source_identifier":"13432133","subitem_source_identifier_type":"ISSN"}]},"item_creator":{"attribute_name":"著者","attribute_type":"creator","attribute_value_mlt":[{"creatorNames":[{"creatorName":"岩木, 信喜"}],"nameIdentifiers":[{}]},{"creatorNames":[{"creatorName":"小坂, 圭子"}],"nameIdentifiers":[{}]},{"creatorNames":[{"creatorName":"近藤, 武夫"}],"nameIdentifiers":[{}]},{"creatorNames":[{"creatorName":"羽渕, 由子"}],"nameIdentifiers":[{}]}]},"item_files":{"attribute_name":"ファイル情報","attribute_type":"file","attribute_value_mlt":[{"accessrole":"open_date","date":[{"dateType":"Available","dateValue":"2013-01-28"}],"displaytype":"detail","filename":"klcVISIO030001.pdf","filesize":[{"value":"837.7 kB"}],"format":"application/pdf","licensetype":"license_6","mimetype":"application/pdf","url":{"label":"klcVISIO030001","url":"https://klc.repo.nii.ac.jp/record/70/files/klcVISIO030001.pdf"},"version_id":"cc3815b8-7931-4f52-a2ee-1efd76e78ee0"}]},"item_language":{"attribute_name":"言語","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_language":"jpn"}]},"item_resource_type":{"attribute_name":"資源タイプ","attribute_value_mlt":[{"resourcetype":"departmental bulletin paper","resourceuri":"http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501"}]},"item_title":"互恵的利他行動文脈が記憶探索時間に及ぼす影響","item_titles":{"attribute_name":"タイトル","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_title":"互恵的利他行動文脈が記憶探索時間に及ぼす影響"},{"subitem_title":"Influence of the Context of Reciprocal Altruism on Memory Search Time","subitem_title_language":"en"}]},"item_type_id":"10002","owner":"13","path":["3"],"pubdate":{"attribute_name":"公開日","attribute_value":"2013-01-28"},"publish_date":"2013-01-28","publish_status":"0","recid":"70","relation_version_is_last":true,"title":["互恵的利他行動文脈が記憶探索時間に及ぼす影響"],"weko_creator_id":"13","weko_shared_id":13},"updated":"2023-06-20T15:07:04.805448+00:00"}